www狠狠干-www日本免费-www三级-www色在线-亚洲午夜网站-亚洲午夜小视频

雅思閱讀真題文章:Ants

雕龍文庫 分享 時間: 收藏本文

雅思閱讀真題文章:Ants

  下面雅思為大家整理了一篇關于ants的雅思閱讀真題文章,這篇雅思閱讀真題的主要內容是討論而來雖然單個的螞蟻可能不那么聰明,但是一群螞蟻的智慧是不容小覷的。斯坦福大學的相關研究人員對此加以研究,下面是詳細內容。

  Collective intelligence::Ants and brains neurons

  STANFORD - An individual ant is not very bright, but ants in a colony, operating as a collective, do remarkable things.

  A single neuron in the human brain can respond only to what the neurons connected to it are doing, but all of them together can be Immanuel Kant.

  That resemblance is why Deborah M. Gordon, StanfordUniversity assistant professor of biological sciences, studies ants.

  39;m interested in the kind of system where simple units together do behave in complicated ways, she said.

  No one gives orders in an ant colony, yet each ant decides what to do next.

  For instance, an ant may have several job descriptions. When the colony discovers a new source of food, an ant doing housekeeping duty may suddenly become a forager. Or if the colonys territory size expands or contracts, patroller ants change the shape of their reconnaissance pattern to conform to the new realities. Since no one is in charge of an ant colony - including the misnamed queen, which is simply a breeder - how does each ant decide what to do?

  This kind of undirected behavior is not unique to ants, Gordon said. How do birds flying in a flock know when to make a collective right turn? All anchovies and other schooling fish seem to turn in unison, yet no one fish is the leader.

  Gordon studies harvester ants in Arizona and, both in the field and in her lab, the so-called Argentine ants that are ubiquitous to coastal California.

  Argentine ants came to Louisiana in a sugar shipment in 1908. They were driven out of the Gulf states by the fire ant and invaded California, where they have displaced most of the native ant species. One of the things Gordon is studying is how they did so. No one has ever seen an ant war involving the Argentine species and the native species, so its not clear whether they are quietly aggressive or just find ways of taking over food resources and territory.

  The Argentine ants in her lab also are being studied to help her understand how they change behavior as the size of the space they are exploring varies.

  The ants are good at finding new places to live in and good at finding food, Gordon said. Were interested in finding out how they do it.

  Her ants are confined by Plexiglas walls and a nasty glue-like substance along the tops of the boards that keeps the ants inside. She moves the walls in and out to change the arena and videotapes the ants movements. A computer tracks each ant from its image on the tape and reads its position so she has a diagram of the ants activities.

  The motions of the ants confirm the existence of a collective.

  A colony is analogous to a brain where there are lots of neurons, each of which can only do something very simple, but together the whole brain can think. None of the neurons can think ant, but the brain can think ant, though nothing in the brain told that neuron to think ant.

  For instance, ants scout for food in a precise pattern. What happens when that pattern no longer fits the circumstances, such as when Gordon moves the walls?

  Ants communicate by chemicals, she said. Thats how they mostly perceive the world; they dont see very well. They use their antennae to smell. So to smell something, they have to get very close to it.

  The best possible way for ants to find everything - if you think of the colony as an individual that is trying to do this - is to have an ant everywhere all the time, because if it doesnt happen close to an ant, theyre not going to know about it. Of course, there are not enough ants in the colony to do that, so somehow the ants have to move around in a pattern that allows them to cover space efficiently.

  Keeping in mind that no one is in charge of a colony and that there is no central plan, how do the ants adjust their reconnaissance if their territory expands or shrinks?

  No ant told them, OK, guys, if the arena is 20 by 20. . . . Somehow there has to be some rule that individual ants use in deciding to change the shape of their paths so they cover the areas effectively. I think that that rule is the rate in which they bump into each other.

  The more crowded they are, the more often each ant will bump into another ant. If the area of their territory is expanded, the frequency of contact decreases. Perhaps, Gordon thinks, each ant has a threshold for normality and adjusts its path shape depending on how often the number of encounters exceeds or falls short of that threshold.

  If the territory shrinks, the number of contacts increases and the ant alters its search pattern. If it expands, contact decreases and it alters the pattern a different way.

  In the Arizona harvester ants, Gordon studies tasks besides patrolling. Each ant has a job.

  I divide the tasks into four: foraging, nest maintenance, midden and patrolling - patrollers are the ones that come out first in the morning and look for food. The foragers go where the patrollers find food.

  The colony has about eight different foraging paths. Every day it uses several of them. The patrollers go out first on the trails and they attract each other when they find food. By the end of an hours patrolling, most patrollers are on just a few trails. . . . All the foragers have to do is go where there are the most patrollers.

  Each ant has its prescribed task, but the ants can switch tasks if the collective needs it. An ant on housekeeping duty will decide to forage. No one told it to do so and Gordon and other entomologists dont know how that happens.

  No ant can possibly know how much food everybody is collecting, how many foragers are needed, she said. An ant has to have very simple rules that tell it, OK, switch and start foraging. But an ant cant assess globally how much food the colony needs.

  39;ve done perturbation experiments in which I marked ants according to what task theyre doing on a given day. The ants that were foraging for food were green, those that were cleaning the nest were blue and so on. Then I created some new situation in the environment; for example, I create a mess that the nest maintenance workers have to clean up or Ill put out extra food that attracts more foragers.

  It turns out that ants that were marked doing a certain task one day switch to do a different task when conditions change.

  Of about 8,000 species of ants, only about 10 percent have been studied thus far.

  Its hard to generalize anything about the behavior of ants, Gordon said. Most of what we know about ants is true of a very, very small number of species compared to the number of species out there.

  上面就是這篇關于ants的雅思閱讀真題文章的全部內容,大家可以從這篇文章中,看到一些非常專業的,關于生物方面的詞匯,雖然這些詞匯不會成為考試的考點,但是了解一下相關的背景知識還是很有好處的。最后,雅思預祝大家在雅思考試中取得好成績!

  

  下面雅思為大家整理了一篇關于ants的雅思閱讀真題文章,這篇雅思閱讀真題的主要內容是討論而來雖然單個的螞蟻可能不那么聰明,但是一群螞蟻的智慧是不容小覷的。斯坦福大學的相關研究人員對此加以研究,下面是詳細內容。

  Collective intelligence::Ants and brains neurons

  STANFORD - An individual ant is not very bright, but ants in a colony, operating as a collective, do remarkable things.

  A single neuron in the human brain can respond only to what the neurons connected to it are doing, but all of them together can be Immanuel Kant.

  That resemblance is why Deborah M. Gordon, StanfordUniversity assistant professor of biological sciences, studies ants.

  39;m interested in the kind of system where simple units together do behave in complicated ways, she said.

  No one gives orders in an ant colony, yet each ant decides what to do next.

  For instance, an ant may have several job descriptions. When the colony discovers a new source of food, an ant doing housekeeping duty may suddenly become a forager. Or if the colonys territory size expands or contracts, patroller ants change the shape of their reconnaissance pattern to conform to the new realities. Since no one is in charge of an ant colony - including the misnamed queen, which is simply a breeder - how does each ant decide what to do?

  This kind of undirected behavior is not unique to ants, Gordon said. How do birds flying in a flock know when to make a collective right turn? All anchovies and other schooling fish seem to turn in unison, yet no one fish is the leader.

  Gordon studies harvester ants in Arizona and, both in the field and in her lab, the so-called Argentine ants that are ubiquitous to coastal California.

  Argentine ants came to Louisiana in a sugar shipment in 1908. They were driven out of the Gulf states by the fire ant and invaded California, where they have displaced most of the native ant species. One of the things Gordon is studying is how they did so. No one has ever seen an ant war involving the Argentine species and the native species, so its not clear whether they are quietly aggressive or just find ways of taking over food resources and territory.

  The Argentine ants in her lab also are being studied to help her understand how they change behavior as the size of the space they are exploring varies.

  The ants are good at finding new places to live in and good at finding food, Gordon said. Were interested in finding out how they do it.

  Her ants are confined by Plexiglas walls and a nasty glue-like substance along the tops of the boards that keeps the ants inside. She moves the walls in and out to change the arena and videotapes the ants movements. A computer tracks each ant from its image on the tape and reads its position so she has a diagram of the ants activities.

  The motions of the ants confirm the existence of a collective.

  A colony is analogous to a brain where there are lots of neurons, each of which can only do something very simple, but together the whole brain can think. None of the neurons can think ant, but the brain can think ant, though nothing in the brain told that neuron to think ant.

  For instance, ants scout for food in a precise pattern. What happens when that pattern no longer fits the circumstances, such as when Gordon moves the walls?

  Ants communicate by chemicals, she said. Thats how they mostly perceive the world; they dont see very well. They use their antennae to smell. So to smell something, they have to get very close to it.

  The best possible way for ants to find everything - if you think of the colony as an individual that is trying to do this - is to have an ant everywhere all the time, because if it doesnt happen close to an ant, theyre not going to know about it. Of course, there are not enough ants in the colony to do that, so somehow the ants have to move around in a pattern that allows them to cover space efficiently.

  Keeping in mind that no one is in charge of a colony and that there is no central plan, how do the ants adjust their reconnaissance if their territory expands or shrinks?

  No ant told them, OK, guys, if the arena is 20 by 20. . . . Somehow there has to be some rule that individual ants use in deciding to change the shape of their paths so they cover the areas effectively. I think that that rule is the rate in which they bump into each other.

  The more crowded they are, the more often each ant will bump into another ant. If the area of their territory is expanded, the frequency of contact decreases. Perhaps, Gordon thinks, each ant has a threshold for normality and adjusts its path shape depending on how often the number of encounters exceeds or falls short of that threshold.

  If the territory shrinks, the number of contacts increases and the ant alters its search pattern. If it expands, contact decreases and it alters the pattern a different way.

  In the Arizona harvester ants, Gordon studies tasks besides patrolling. Each ant has a job.

  I divide the tasks into four: foraging, nest maintenance, midden and patrolling - patrollers are the ones that come out first in the morning and look for food. The foragers go where the patrollers find food.

  The colony has about eight different foraging paths. Every day it uses several of them. The patrollers go out first on the trails and they attract each other when they find food. By the end of an hours patrolling, most patrollers are on just a few trails. . . . All the foragers have to do is go where there are the most patrollers.

  Each ant has its prescribed task, but the ants can switch tasks if the collective needs it. An ant on housekeeping duty will decide to forage. No one told it to do so and Gordon and other entomologists dont know how that happens.

  No ant can possibly know how much food everybody is collecting, how many foragers are needed, she said. An ant has to have very simple rules that tell it, OK, switch and start foraging. But an ant cant assess globally how much food the colony needs.

  39;ve done perturbation experiments in which I marked ants according to what task theyre doing on a given day. The ants that were foraging for food were green, those that were cleaning the nest were blue and so on. Then I created some new situation in the environment; for example, I create a mess that the nest maintenance workers have to clean up or Ill put out extra food that attracts more foragers.

  It turns out that ants that were marked doing a certain task one day switch to do a different task when conditions change.

  Of about 8,000 species of ants, only about 10 percent have been studied thus far.

  Its hard to generalize anything about the behavior of ants, Gordon said. Most of what we know about ants is true of a very, very small number of species compared to the number of species out there.

  上面就是這篇關于ants的雅思閱讀真題文章的全部內容,大家可以從這篇文章中,看到一些非常專業的,關于生物方面的詞匯,雖然這些詞匯不會成為考試的考點,但是了解一下相關的背景知識還是很有好處的。最后,雅思預祝大家在雅思考試中取得好成績!

  

主站蜘蛛池模板: 国产自啪啪| 亚洲毛片在线观看 | 国产精品嫩草影院88v | 天天操操操操操操 | 最近的2019中文字幕免费一页 | 国产午夜亚洲精品国产 | 精品欧美一区二区三区 | 久久综合精品不卡一区二区 | 亚洲欧美一区二区久久 | 日韩手机在线 | 毛片在线播放视频 | 天天看天天色 | 亚洲欧美日韩综合二区三区 | 一区二区三区在线播放 | 久久久免费的精品 | 免费一级成人免费观看 | 黄色一级录像片 | 午夜激情一区 | 日韩αv| 成人国产精品视频频 | 男人下面进女人下面视频免费 | 国产精品一区二区在线观看 | 欧美成人性性 | 中国毛片视频 | 亚洲黄色网址在线观看 | 日韩字幕一中文在线综合 | 日韩天堂视频 | 欧美在线视频免费 | 一级一级一级毛片 | 深夜福利视频网 | 全免费午夜一级毛片真人 | 五月香婷婷 | 国产成人一区二区三区影院免费 | 日韩欧美亚洲一区精选 | 最近最新中文字幕免费高清1 | 精品导航 | 亚洲最新中文字幕 | 宅男在线午夜影院 | 国产精品国产三级在线高清观看 | 欧美aav| 国产日韩在线视频 |